The objective of this study was to determine the effects of patient-based mammography screening strategies. We performed a meta-analysis and included United States studies that met the following criteria: (a) randomized or concurrent control design; (b) defined outcomes; and (c) data available for reanalysis. Interventions were classified as behavioral, cognitive, or sociological and further categorized by the type of control group (active versus usual care), number of interventions, and mode of intervention (interactive versus static). Data were combined using DerSimonian and Laird random effects models to yield summary effect sizes. A total of 63 interventions in 43 studies met the inclusion criteria. Behavioral interventions increased screening by 13.2% [95% confidence interval (CI), 4.7–21.2] compared with usual care, and by 13.0% (95% CI, 8.7–17.4) when using multiple strategies and 5.6% (95% CI, 0.6–10.6) when using a single intervention compared to active controls. Cognitive interventions using generic education strategies had little impact on screening, but those that used theory-based education (e.g., health belief model) increased rates by 23.6% (95% CI, 16.4–30.1) compared with usual care. Sociological interventions also increased screening rates. Interventions using a theoretical framework were the most effective in increasing screening rates. The ability of these interventions to increase screening among subgroups and improve rates of ongoing screening, as well as the costs of these strategies, is unknown and is an important area for future research.

Despite evidence that regular mammography screening can reduce breast cancer mortality (1, 2, 3), many women fail to receive mammography or adhere to recommended guidelines for routine ongoing screening. In the United States, women who do not receive regular mammography are more likely to be elderly (4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10), uninsured or underinsured (10, 11), lack a usual source of care (12, 13, 14, 15), have lower levels of education or income (10, 16, 17, 18, 19), be non-white, (12, 13, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24), be non-native English speakers (25), or have low levels of literacy (12, 26, 27).

When asked why they do not receive screening, women report that breast cancer screening tests are unnecessary in the absence of symptoms (22, 28), that they do not believe themselves to be at risk of cancer (29), or that they are concerned about inconvenience, discomfort, trouble, embarrassment, or pain (27, 30). Culturally mediated concerns about modesty or dignity as well as fatalistic attitudes toward cancer may also prevent women from seeking screening (31, 32). Lower rates of mammography utilization may also result from practical considerations; in rural areas or areas that lack mammography facilities, women are less likely to receive screening (33, 34, 35). Although one of the strongest predictors of mammography screening is physician recommendation (12, 28), compliance with such a recommendation may be complicated by distrust of the medical profession in some patient populations (12, 28).

During the past two decades, numerous studies have described interventions developed to address the aforementioned patient barriers to screening (36). However, because of the large number of different interventions, numerous mechanisms of intervention action, variability in study design, and, in some cases, small sample size, it is difficult to develop a cohesive public health approach to improving screening use, particularly in high-risk populations. In this study, we perform a critical review of well-designed patient-targeted interventions designed to increase adherence with mammography. We estimate overall effect sizes for specific types of interventions to determine the most effective strategies.

Study Selection.

We used the OVID search mechanism with MEDLINE in the years 1980–1998 to identify published English language articles on interventions to increase mammography utilization. The search strategy was as follows: we used the terms “mammography” or “breast neoplasms/prevention and control” (n = 7,074) to identify the subset of studies focused on mammography screening. We then developed a series of terms to identify settings in which interventions could take place (e.g., “primary health care,” “gynecology,” and “family physicians”) and used the terms “health education,” “health behavior,” “patient compliance,” “patient acceptance of health care,” “attitude to health,” or “health promotion” (n = 144,660).

The combination of these searches yielded 600 studies. Study abstracts were reviewed for evidence of prospective follow-up with either randomized assignment to an intervention or control group or a concurrent control group. Pre/post designs without controls and uncontrolled trials were excluded. Increasing rates of mammography use in the general population over the past two decades (37) complicate the interpretation of studies without prospective control groups. Any observed increase in mammography use might be attributable to the secular trend rather than the intervention under evaluation. Published abstracts were also excluded because they were judged to have too brief a description of methods for assessment.

Forty-eight patient-targeted studies met these criteria; four of these studies were subsequently eliminated because they lacked concurrent control groups (38, 39, 40, 41), and three other studies were eliminated because the intervention was not described in sufficient detail for classification (42, 43, 44). This left 41 studies identified from the literature search. Reference lists of the selected studies were also searched to identify other eligible studies, and a hand search of the Journal of General Internal Medicine, Medical Care, Preventive Medicine, Annals of Internal Medicine, Archives of Internal Medicine, and Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers and Prevention was conducted for June-August 1998. Ten additional studies were thus identified, yielding a total of 51 studies, 43 of which were performed in the United States. Studies performed outside the United States were excluded because underlying differences in health care systems might limit their generalizability (45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52). Interventions performed in populations of women selected at the receipt of mammography (intervention to increase repeat screening) are included in the data tables (53, 54, 55) but were excluded from quantitative analyses.

Data Abstraction.

We classified interventions as cognitive, behavioral, or sociological (56). Cognitive strategies provide new information and education, increase existing knowledge, and clarify misperceptions. Behavioral interventions alter cues or stimuli associated with screening behavior; and sociological interventions use social norms or peers to increase screening adherence. Interventions were further classified by the type of control group used, active control and usual care control. Active control groups included a lower level of an intervention, such as generic educational material describing population risk of breast cancer, mailed and telephone reminders, flow sheets in patient charts, and videotaped education. We defined usual care controls as situations in which no intervention associated with mammography utilization was performed. In settings where usual care included routine reminders or flow sheets in patient charts, these interventions were classified as having an active control group.

Within groups of interventions classified by category and type of control group, where a sufficient number of interventions were available, we further classified interventions by the number of strategies (single versus multiple) and mode of delivery (e.g., interactive delivery of intervention via telephone or in-person versus static delivery of intervention through letters or videotapes).

Data were abstracted from studies using a standardized abstraction format to describe the type of intervention, characteristics of mammography outcome determination (self-report, medical charts, electronic records, or medical claims), the patient population, and intervention effectiveness. Studies with multiple, separate interventions had these data abstracted when possible. Additionally, for studies with multiple assessment points over time, the first assessment was abstracted and used in the combined analysis.

Data Analysis.

Effect sizes and 95% CIs3 were calculated for each intervention. For randomized studies, intervention effectiveness was calculated as the difference in mammography utilization between the intervention and control group at the end of the study. Variance was calculated using the following formula.

For concurrently controlled studies, the effect size was calculated as the difference between the rates post- and preintervention for the intervention group and the control group. The variance was calculated using the following formula.

Among groups of interventions, these effect sizes and 95% CIs were graphed and inspected visually for homogeneity. Formal tests of homogeneity, the DerSimonian and Laird Q-statistic (57), were performed for interventions grouped by mechanism of action (behavioral, cognitive, or sociological), the type of control group, and the mode of intervention delivery. This statistic compares the summary effect measure and the within-study effect measure. It was compared against a χ2 distribution with a null hypothesis of homogeneity. Meta-Analyst software (58) was used to calculate the combined effect size using random effects models. These models incorporate deviation due to statistical error as well as that due to any underlying differences in study population or other factors specific to individual studies. DerSimonian and Laird random effects summary statistics and 95% CIs (57) are reported separately for different types of interventions. To test the influence of a single intervention on the summarized results, we performed sensitivity analysis by sequentially dropping each intervention and recalculating the summary statistics.

Among the 43 patient-targeted studies in the final study sample, there were 63 distinct interventions to increase mammography utilization: (a) 27 behavioral interventions; (b) 21 cognitive interventions; and (c) 9 sociological interventions; and (d) 5 interventions that used both cognitive and behavioral strategies (Table 1; Refs. 53, 54, 55 and 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 72, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 96, 97, 98). Mammography outcomes were assessed most frequently by self-report (49%), followed by medical chart audit, medical claims or electronic records, and radiology facility reports. The majority of the study populations were composed of white women and women ages 50 years and over. In the studies that reported previous mammography use, the most frequent rates of ever having previous mammography were between 26% and 50%.

Patient-targeted behavioral interventions are listed in Table 2(53, 54, 55, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 97, 98) and include telephone reminders (61, 63) and letters from clinical program directors or primary care clinicians (49, 55, 59, 61, 63, 66, 67). A total of 40% used usual care comparison groups, and 60% had active comparison groups.

Overall, in six interventions with a usual care control (54, 59, 61, 62, 63), 13.2% more women received mammography after receiving a behavioral intervention than women getting usual care (95% CI, 4.7–21.2). The high Q-statistic (19.3) and the wide CI indicate that these studies are heterogeneous. Sensitivity analyses with sequential removal of each intervention did not change this estimate, indicating that heterogeneity was not associated with a single study.

When the seven behavioral interventions using active controls and a single intervention strategy (64, 65, 66, 68, 98) were combined, the null hypothesis of homogeneity was rejected (Q = 33.3). The heterogeneity was associated with a single study (65), and when it was removed, the combined studies were considered homogeneous (Q = 14.3). The overall rate of mammography was 5.5% higher for women receiving behavioral interventions compared with active controls (95% CI, 0.6 to 10.6). Sensitivity analyses with sequential removal of each intervention did little to affect the size of the summary estimate, but did impact the statistical significance.

The combination of six behavioral, multiple-part interventions with active controls was homogeneous (Q = 5.6; Refs. 54, 66, 67, 69, 71, and 97). The combined estimate for increased mammography utilization was 13.0% (95% CI, 8.6–17.4).

Interventions based on theories of cognitive change typically identify patient attitudes to screening and breast cancer and provide focused educational material directed at increasing compliance with mammography. For example, under the health belief model, a woman is likely to undergo screening mammography if she believes that she is susceptible to breast cancer (perceived susceptibility), that consequences of breast cancer are severe (perceived severity), that mammography has benefits in terms of reducing the impact of breast cancer (perceived benefits), and that barriers associated with receiving a mammography are low (perceived barriers; Ref. 100).

Based on differences in the approach of the cognitive interventions, we further classified cognitive strategies into generic patient education listed in Table 3(72, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77) and theory-based education listed in Table 4(67, 68, 70, 74, 75, 86, 87, 88, 89, 90, 97).

There were seven interventions that compared generic patient education strategies to usual care. These interventions were homogeneous (Q = 7.3), and although several led to an increase in mammography screening (72, 73, 75, 76, 77), overall, this effect was small (1.1%) and was not significant (95% CI, −2.4 to 4.6). Sensitivity analyses did not affect this interpretation.

We divided theory-based cognitive interventions into two groups based on the type of comparison group. Interventions that used usual care controls were homogeneous (Q = 4.1), and compared with usual care, theory-based cognitive interventions appear to be very effective in increasing the rate of mammography utilization: overall, 23.6% more women received mammography (95% CI, 16.4–30.1; Refs. (75, 88).

We divided theory-based cognitive interventions with active controls into those that were static [delivered by letter or videotape (74, 86, 87, 89)], and those that were interactive [delivered by telephone or in person (67, 68, 70, 90, 97)]. The interventions delivered by letter or videotape were homogeneous (Q = 6.3) but were ineffective, with an estimated increase in mammography utilization of less than 1% (0.4; 95% CI, -5.4 to 6.2). However, theory-based cognitive interventions delivered interactively were effective, with a combined increase in mammography utilization of 7.9% (95% CI, 2.3–13.5). Sensitivity analyses did not change the interpretation of either of these estimates (see Table 4).

We identified nine sociological interventions to increase mammography screening (Refs. 77, 78, 79, 80, 82, 83, 84, 85, and , 91; Table 5). These patient-targeted sociological interventions used community peers (78, 83, 91), friends (80), lay health advisors (82, 84, 85), or media representations (91) of appropriate behavior to influence screening behaviors. The studies that used interactive sociological interventions were relatively homogeneous (Q = 19.8; Refs. 77, 78, 79, 80 and 82, 83, 84, 85) and improved mammography utilization by 12.6% (95% CI, 7.4–17.9). Sensitivity analyses had little impact on this result.

We found two patient-targeted interventions that used financial incentives to try to increase mammography utilization (92, 93). Both led to increases in mammography utilization, but we could not perform meta-analysis with two interventions.

We also identified five interventions (70, 94, 95, 96, 97) that used both behavioral and cognitive strategies to increase mammography utilization. These interventions had variable effectiveness, ranging from little effect (70, 95, 97) to a maximum effect of 33% (94).

In our meta-analyses of patient-targeted interventions to increase mammography utilization, we found that most interventions increased rates of screening. Behavioral interventions increased screening by 13.2% (95% CI, 4.7–21.2) compared with usual care, by 13.0% (95% CI, 8.6–17.4) when using multiple strategies, and by 5.6% (95% CI, 0.6–10.6) when using a single intervention compared with active controls. Cognitive interventions using generic education strategies had little impact on screening, but those that used theory-based education (e.g., health belief model) increased screening rates by 23.6% (95% CI, 16.4–30.1) compared with usual care. Sociological interventions also increased screening rates.

The mode of intervention delivery appears to be an important component in increasing rates of mammography utilization. In particular, multiple behavioral interventions (e.g., two reminder letters) led to improved rates of mammography utilization compared with active controls, but a single behavioral intervention had a minimal effect when compared with active controls. Similarly, theory-based cognitive interventions delivered interactively via telephone or in person led to improved rates of mammography screening compared with active controls, whereas theory-based cognitive interventions delivered through letter or videotape had little impact on mammography utilization compared to active controls. The combined sociological interventions, also delivered interactively, were very effective.

These results have important public health implications for the design and delivery of interventions to increase mammography screening. Recent national estimates indicate that 56% of women over the age of 50 years have received a screening mammogram within the past 2 years (10). With the use of theory-based behavioral interventions, this number could be increased to 73–86%, with a resultant down-staging of disease and an improvement in morbidity and mortality.

Similar to our findings, in a recent review of mailed patient reminders, Wagner (100) reported that these interventions led to increased mammography utilization when compared with no intervention or a generic letter.

The results of our meta-analyses were relatively robust. By sequentially removing each study, we could determine the independent impact of a single study on overall results. In no case did the removal of a single study have a large impact on the summarized estimate, and the interpretation of the effect changed only once.

All of the interventions reviewed here included control groups of similar women and were grouped according to mechanism of intervention action. However, there are important differences in the women enrolled in the different types of interventions, which may affect the interpretation and comparison of these results. For example, most of the patients included in the behavioral interventions had health insurance or at least a usual source of care from which a list of potentially eligible patients could be developed (61, 62, 66, 67, 69, 70). Additionally, some studies included populations of women with high rates of previous mammography (62, 79), yet many of the patients included in the sociological interventions did not have health insurance (85, 91) or had low rates of previous mammography (82, 83, 84, 91). These health care utilization characteristics, which are associated with mammography utilization in cross-sectional studies (10, 11, 12, 13, 15), may also affect patient responsiveness to interventions to increase screening. Thus, comparisons among interventions and adaptation of interventions to dissimilar populations should be approached cautiously. Planning of new interventions should consider existing strategies in the target population, population characteristics, and available resources for intervention delivery.

There are some methodological limitations with the meta-analysis reported here, including heterogeneity among studies that were combined, inconsistencies in the unit of analysis used to calculate intervention effectiveness, reporting of multiple interventions per study with a single control group, and the combination of multiple measures of mammography utilization. We combined data from studies conducted in dissimilar populations or environments in which mammography screening is obtained and attempted to make the groups of interventions as homogeneous as possible, yet in several cases, our measures of homogeneity were high, causing us to reject the null hypothesis that interventions were homogenous. However, sensitivity analyses for these groups indicated that there was no one study that was influencing the results. Overall, combining heterogenous studies will produce a summary estimate that is biased toward no effect.

The majority of studies we identified randomized individual women to receive either the intervention or control condition. However, some studies randomized women by physician or practice group (63, 72, 83, 98) or retirement communities (94, 97). Additionally, related individuals, such as friends of volunteers (79), or women’s groups (88) were used as a source of subjects, and peer counselors delivered interventions to multiple women (80, 82, 83, 84, 85). Yet these data are combined as if randomization occurred individually and all observations are independent. Women treated by the same physician, living in the same retirement community, or who received tailored interventions from the same peer counselor are likely to be more similar in terms of mammography utilization than those recruited from a random sample or those that received a standardized intervention. If analyses were to incorporate the actual unit of analysis (e.g., practice group, retirement community) or correlation among individuals, CIs would be wider, but the estimate of intervention effectiveness would remain unchanged.

The impact of including data from multiple interventions that used a single control group in our summarized estimates is also likely to result in an overstatement of intervention effectiveness via narrower CIs.

The studies included here used several mechanisms to identify mammography utilization: (a) self-report; (b) chart audit; (c) electronic claims; and (d) mammography facility records. We considered the validity of these different sources to be equivalent, although this is not necessarily the case. Self-report of mammography has been described as highly correlated with mammography use reported in patient charts or claims, but it is likely to overstate utilization (101, 102), particularly among low-income minority populations (103). However, women in intervention and control groups might be equally likely to overstate mammography utilization, so the relative estimate of mammography utilization (intervention versus control) is unlikely to be influenced by the reporting source.

There are some more general limitations that affect the interpretation of the meta-analyses presented here. The studies identified and included were based on a review of the published literature. Studies with negative or null findings might be less likely to be published and thus less likely to be included in this review. This would result in an overstatement of the effectiveness of interventions to improve rates of mammography screening.

Finally, improvements in mammography utilization at a single point in time as described in the studies here do not translate directly into reductions in morbidity and mortality from breast cancer. Even if women do receive regular screening, it is possible that as a result of delays in follow-up after an abnormal test result, incomplete diagnostic work-up, or the lack of adherence to a treatment regimen, a reduction in morbidity and mortality may not be realized. Additionally, there may be adverse effects associated with interventions to increase mammography utilization such as increased rates of false positive exams, which are estimated to be as high as 30% among women receiving regular mammography over a 10-year period (104), and associated psychological distress (105).

Overall, behavioral interventions, theory-based cognitive interventions, and sociological patient-targeted interventions appear to be effective in increasing mammography utilization, particularly when compared with usual care. Multiple behavioral interventions and interactive theory-based cognitive interventions are effective when compared with active controls. The long-term effectiveness of these interventions in increasing rates of regular mammography is only rarely reported (60, 77), and this will be an important area for further research. Additionally, the effectiveness of different types of interventions in patient subpopulations such as elderly, minority, or low-income women and the costs of providing these interventions are critical areas for research in decreasing the morbidity and mortality associated with breast cancer.

The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked advertisement in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

        
1

Supported in part by Agency for Health Care Policy and Research Grant HS 08395, Grant DAMD17-94-J-4212 from the Department of the Army, a Breast Cancer Center grant (to J. S. M.), and National Cancer Institute Grants RO1 CA72908 (to J. S. M. and K. R. Y.) and T32 CA09314-19 (to K. R. Y.). An earlier version of this work was part of a 1999 report prepared for the Institute of Medicine entitled Access to Quality Cancer Care: Evaluating and Ensuring Equitable Services, Quality of Life, and Survival.

                
3

The abbreviation used is: CI, confidence interval.

Table 1

Intervention characteristics

No.Percentage (%)Reference no.
Behavioral intervention 11 17.5 53, 54, 59–63 
 Usual care    
 Active control 16 25.4 54, 55, 64–71, 97 
Cognitive intervention    
 Generic education 11.1 72–77 
 Theory-based education    
  Usual care control 63 75, 88 
  Active control 10 15.9 67, 68, 70, 74, 86, 87, 89, 90, 97 
Sociological intervention 14.3 77–85, 91 
Financial incentives 3.2 92 and 93 
Behavioral and cognitive intervention 6.3 70, 94–97 
Total interventions 63 100.0  
No. of intervention modalities    
 Single 15 34.9 53, 54, 60, 63, 76, 78, 79, 82, 87, 89, 93, 95; 64, 84, 85 
 Multiple 28 65.1 46, 55, 59, 61–63, 65–70, 98; 72–75, 77, 80, 83, 86, 88, 90–92, 94, 97; 96 
Study design    
 Randomized controlled trial 40 93.0 45, 53, 54, 61, 62, 65–70, 74, 95; 75–78, 80, 84, 87, 89, 90, 92, 93 48, 63, 72, 79, 83, 85, 86, 88, 96–98; 55, 59, 64, 94 
 Concurrently controlled trials 60, 73, 91 
Mammography utilization    
 Self-report 21 48.8 53, 67, 70, 73, 75, 76, 86–90, 93, 94, 97 
 Chart audit 20.9 78, 79, 82, 84, 85, 91, 92 
   53, 60, 61, 65, 72, 83, 84, 93, 95 
 Claims/eletronic record 18.6 52, 55, 62, 63, 66, 69, 74, 77, 80 
 Mammography facility report/record 9.3 46, 50, 51, 54, 59, 68, 80 
 Mammography appointment 2.3 98 
Patient age group (yrs)a    
 <40 11.6 55, 71, 73, 75, 79, 88 
 40–49 20 46.5 53, 55, 60, 62, 63, 72, 75–77, 86–88, 90 
   71, 79, 82, 84, 85, 89, 91 
 50–59 31 72.1 53–55, 60, 61, 63, 65–70, 95; 72, 74–77, 86–88, 90 
   48, 64, 67, 71, 79, 82, 83, 85, 91, 92 
 >60 26 60.5 52, 53, 55, 59, 60, 63, 65–67, 69, 70, 95, 97 
   72, 75–77, 79, 80, 83, 85, 88, 90, 91, 93, 94 
   71 
 Not stated 2.3 98 
Patient racea    
 ≥20% Black 20.9 62, 64, 65, 77, 79, 82, 83, 93, 97, 98 
 ≥20% Hispanic 2.3 85 and 91 
 ≥20% White 22 51.2 59, 61, 63, 64, 67, 72, 73, 75–77, 88, 94, 97 
   71, 79, 80, 83, 84, 86, 87, 89, 90, 92, 96 
 ≥20% Asian 2.3 71 
 Not stated 14 32.6 53–55, 60, 66, 68, 69, 74, 78, 95 
   70 
Percentage with health insurancea    
 <50% 7.0 68, 85, 91 
 50–74% 4.7 59, 65 
 75–99% 16.3 61, 72, 83, 84, 87, 92, 97 
 100% 11 25.6 62, 63, 66, 67, 69, 70, 73, 74, 93–95 
 Not stated 20 46.5 53–55, 60, 75–78, 86, 88–90, 99; 64, 79, 80, 96 
Percentage with previous mammography    
 0–25% 9.3 65, 74, 83, 91 
 26–50% 12 27.9 50, 60, 66, 72, 73, 82, 84, 85, 87, 88, 93, 94, 96 
 51–75% 14.0 73, 76, 78, 80, 93, 97 
 76–99% 7.0 62, 79 
 100% 7.0 53–55 
 Not stated 15 34.9 46, 49, 59, 61, 63, 67–70, 75, 90, 95, 98; 64, 71, 77, 86, 89, 92 
No.Percentage (%)Reference no.
Behavioral intervention 11 17.5 53, 54, 59–63 
 Usual care    
 Active control 16 25.4 54, 55, 64–71, 97 
Cognitive intervention    
 Generic education 11.1 72–77 
 Theory-based education    
  Usual care control 63 75, 88 
  Active control 10 15.9 67, 68, 70, 74, 86, 87, 89, 90, 97 
Sociological intervention 14.3 77–85, 91 
Financial incentives 3.2 92 and 93 
Behavioral and cognitive intervention 6.3 70, 94–97 
Total interventions 63 100.0  
No. of intervention modalities    
 Single 15 34.9 53, 54, 60, 63, 76, 78, 79, 82, 87, 89, 93, 95; 64, 84, 85 
 Multiple 28 65.1 46, 55, 59, 61–63, 65–70, 98; 72–75, 77, 80, 83, 86, 88, 90–92, 94, 97; 96 
Study design    
 Randomized controlled trial 40 93.0 45, 53, 54, 61, 62, 65–70, 74, 95; 75–78, 80, 84, 87, 89, 90, 92, 93 48, 63, 72, 79, 83, 85, 86, 88, 96–98; 55, 59, 64, 94 
 Concurrently controlled trials 60, 73, 91 
Mammography utilization    
 Self-report 21 48.8 53, 67, 70, 73, 75, 76, 86–90, 93, 94, 97 
 Chart audit 20.9 78, 79, 82, 84, 85, 91, 92 
   53, 60, 61, 65, 72, 83, 84, 93, 95 
 Claims/eletronic record 18.6 52, 55, 62, 63, 66, 69, 74, 77, 80 
 Mammography facility report/record 9.3 46, 50, 51, 54, 59, 68, 80 
 Mammography appointment 2.3 98 
Patient age group (yrs)a    
 <40 11.6 55, 71, 73, 75, 79, 88 
 40–49 20 46.5 53, 55, 60, 62, 63, 72, 75–77, 86–88, 90 
   71, 79, 82, 84, 85, 89, 91 
 50–59 31 72.1 53–55, 60, 61, 63, 65–70, 95; 72, 74–77, 86–88, 90 
   48, 64, 67, 71, 79, 82, 83, 85, 91, 92 
 >60 26 60.5 52, 53, 55, 59, 60, 63, 65–67, 69, 70, 95, 97 
   72, 75–77, 79, 80, 83, 85, 88, 90, 91, 93, 94 
   71 
 Not stated 2.3 98 
Patient racea    
 ≥20% Black 20.9 62, 64, 65, 77, 79, 82, 83, 93, 97, 98 
 ≥20% Hispanic 2.3 85 and 91 
 ≥20% White 22 51.2 59, 61, 63, 64, 67, 72, 73, 75–77, 88, 94, 97 
   71, 79, 80, 83, 84, 86, 87, 89, 90, 92, 96 
 ≥20% Asian 2.3 71 
 Not stated 14 32.6 53–55, 60, 66, 68, 69, 74, 78, 95 
   70 
Percentage with health insurancea    
 <50% 7.0 68, 85, 91 
 50–74% 4.7 59, 65 
 75–99% 16.3 61, 72, 83, 84, 87, 92, 97 
 100% 11 25.6 62, 63, 66, 67, 69, 70, 73, 74, 93–95 
 Not stated 20 46.5 53–55, 60, 75–78, 86, 88–90, 99; 64, 79, 80, 96 
Percentage with previous mammography    
 0–25% 9.3 65, 74, 83, 91 
 26–50% 12 27.9 50, 60, 66, 72, 73, 82, 84, 85, 87, 88, 93, 94, 96 
 51–75% 14.0 73, 76, 78, 80, 93, 97 
 76–99% 7.0 62, 79 
 100% 7.0 53–55 
 Not stated 15 34.9 46, 49, 59, 61, 63, 67–70, 75, 90, 95, 98; 64, 71, 77, 86, 89, 92 
a

Values may not add up to 100% because some interventions fall into more than one category.

Table 2

Patient-targeted behavioral interventions

Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
Usual care controls 59a Community 41 43 6 (15%) 2 (5%) 10 (−2.7–22.7) 
 60a University NEc NE Pre, 40.9% Pre, 37.3% 7.5 NE 
     Post, 57% Post, 46%   
 53b, d Community 44 48 32 (72.7%) 17 (35.6%) 37.1 (18.2–56.0) 
 53b, d Community 43 48 31 (72.1%) 17 (35.6%) 36.5 (17.4–55.6) 
 53b, d Community 43 48 19 (44.2%) 17 (35.6%) 8.6 (−11.5–28.7) 
 54b, d Community 32 31 15 (47%) 6 (19%) 28 (5.9–50.1) 
 61b Community 38 38 7 (18%) 4 (11%) (−8.8–22.8) 
 61b Community 38 38 11 (29%) 4 (11%) 18 (0.5–35.5) 
 61b Community 37 38 16 (43%) 4 (11%) 32 (13.2–50.8) 
 62b Community 388 381 106 (27.4%) 97 (25.4%) 1.6 (−4.6–7.8) 
 63b Community 337 322 88 (26%) 28 (8.7%) 17.3 (11.7–22.9) 
Summary Q = 19.3      13.2 (4.7–21.2) 
Active controls, single intervention 98b University 98 130 18 (18%) 25 (19%) −1 (−11.2–9.2) 
 65b, e Community 329 266 56 (21.0%) 73 (27.4%) −6 (−13.3–0.5) 
 55a, d Community 50 50 20 (40%) 17 (34%) (−12.9–24.8) 
 55a Community 50 50 27 (54%) 17 (34%) 20 (0.9–39.1) 
 66b Community 329 329 150 (45.6%) 154 (46.8%) −1.2 (−8.8–6.4) 
 66b Community 335 329 196 (58.5%) 154 (46.8%) 11.7 (4.5–18.9) 
 67b Community 381 364 159 (42%) 100 (28%) 14 (7.9–20.1) 
 67b Community 198 198 28 (14%) 23 (12%) (−6.6–8.6) 
 68b Community 384 424 306 (79.7%) 316 (74.5%) 5.2 (−.5–11.0) 
Summary Q = 14.3      5.6 (0.6–10.6) 
Active controls multiple 71b Community 343 344 250 (73%) 186 (54%) 19 (12.1–26.1) 
 66b Community 334 329 206 (61.7%) 154 (46.8%) 14.9 (7.4–22.4) 
 54b, d Community 96 91 31 (32%) 33 (36%) −4 (−17.6–9.6) 
 54b, d Community 92 92 44 (48%) 41 (44%) −10.0–18.4) 
 69b Community 1171 1171 310 (26.5%) 187 (16.0%) 10.5 (7.2–13.8) 
 97b Community 95 122 20 (21%) 16 (13%) 8.0 (−2.1–18.1) 
Summary Q = 5.6      13.0 (8.6–17.4) 
Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
Usual care controls 59a Community 41 43 6 (15%) 2 (5%) 10 (−2.7–22.7) 
 60a University NEc NE Pre, 40.9% Pre, 37.3% 7.5 NE 
     Post, 57% Post, 46%   
 53b, d Community 44 48 32 (72.7%) 17 (35.6%) 37.1 (18.2–56.0) 
 53b, d Community 43 48 31 (72.1%) 17 (35.6%) 36.5 (17.4–55.6) 
 53b, d Community 43 48 19 (44.2%) 17 (35.6%) 8.6 (−11.5–28.7) 
 54b, d Community 32 31 15 (47%) 6 (19%) 28 (5.9–50.1) 
 61b Community 38 38 7 (18%) 4 (11%) (−8.8–22.8) 
 61b Community 38 38 11 (29%) 4 (11%) 18 (0.5–35.5) 
 61b Community 37 38 16 (43%) 4 (11%) 32 (13.2–50.8) 
 62b Community 388 381 106 (27.4%) 97 (25.4%) 1.6 (−4.6–7.8) 
 63b Community 337 322 88 (26%) 28 (8.7%) 17.3 (11.7–22.9) 
Summary Q = 19.3      13.2 (4.7–21.2) 
Active controls, single intervention 98b University 98 130 18 (18%) 25 (19%) −1 (−11.2–9.2) 
 65b, e Community 329 266 56 (21.0%) 73 (27.4%) −6 (−13.3–0.5) 
 55a, d Community 50 50 20 (40%) 17 (34%) (−12.9–24.8) 
 55a Community 50 50 27 (54%) 17 (34%) 20 (0.9–39.1) 
 66b Community 329 329 150 (45.6%) 154 (46.8%) −1.2 (−8.8–6.4) 
 66b Community 335 329 196 (58.5%) 154 (46.8%) 11.7 (4.5–18.9) 
 67b Community 381 364 159 (42%) 100 (28%) 14 (7.9–20.1) 
 67b Community 198 198 28 (14%) 23 (12%) (−6.6–8.6) 
 68b Community 384 424 306 (79.7%) 316 (74.5%) 5.2 (−.5–11.0) 
Summary Q = 14.3      5.6 (0.6–10.6) 
Active controls multiple 71b Community 343 344 250 (73%) 186 (54%) 19 (12.1–26.1) 
 66b Community 334 329 206 (61.7%) 154 (46.8%) 14.9 (7.4–22.4) 
 54b, d Community 96 91 31 (32%) 33 (36%) −4 (−17.6–9.6) 
 54b, d Community 92 92 44 (48%) 41 (44%) −10.0–18.4) 
 69b Community 1171 1171 310 (26.5%) 187 (16.0%) 10.5 (7.2–13.8) 
 97b Community 95 122 20 (21%) 16 (13%) 8.0 (−2.1–18.1) 
Summary Q = 5.6      13.0 (8.6–17.4) 
a

Concurrent control group.

b

Random control group.

c

NE, not evaluable; Pre, preintervention; Post, postintervention.

d

Intervention was performed in a population of women enrolled at receipt of mammography (intervention to increase repeat screening) and excluded from quantitative analysis.

e

Excluded from quantitative analysis.

Table 3

Patient-targeted cognitive interventions—generic patient education

Intervention used performance score to describe screening.
Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
72a University 216 216 104 (48.1%) 97 (44.9%) (−6.4–12.4) 
73b Worksite 384 379 Pre, 46.9%c Pre, 52.8% 4.9 (−4.9–14.7) 
    Post, 62.0% Post, 63%   
74a Community 447 440 110 (24.6%) 121 (27.5%) −2.9 (−8.7–2.9) 
74a Community 595 440 161 (27.1%) 121 (27.5%) −0.4 (−5.9–5.1) 
75a Community 75 78 55 (73%) 48 (62%) 11 (−3.7–0.26) 
76a Community NE NE 44% 31% 13 NE 
77a University 143 144 26 (18%) 31 (21%) −3 (−12.2–6.2) 
        
Summary Q = 7.3     1.1 (−2.4–4.6) 
Intervention used performance score to describe screening.
Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
72a University 216 216 104 (48.1%) 97 (44.9%) (−6.4–12.4) 
73b Worksite 384 379 Pre, 46.9%c Pre, 52.8% 4.9 (−4.9–14.7) 
    Post, 62.0% Post, 63%   
74a Community 447 440 110 (24.6%) 121 (27.5%) −2.9 (−8.7–2.9) 
74a Community 595 440 161 (27.1%) 121 (27.5%) −0.4 (−5.9–5.1) 
75a Community 75 78 55 (73%) 48 (62%) 11 (−3.7–0.26) 
76a Community NE NE 44% 31% 13 NE 
77a University 143 144 26 (18%) 31 (21%) −3 (−12.2–6.2) 
        
Summary Q = 7.3     1.1 (−2.4–4.6) 
a

Random control group.

b

Concurrent control group.

c

Pre, preintervention; Post, postintervention; NE, not evaluable.

Table 4

Patient-targeted cognitive interventions—theory-based patient education

Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
Usual care controls 75a Community 74 78 53 (72%) 48 (62%) 10 (−4.9–24.8) 
 75a Community 73 78 64 (87%) 48 (62%) 25 (11.8–38.0) 
 88a Community 141 122 53 (37.6%) 21 (17.2%) 20.4 (10.0–30.8) 
 88a Community 107 122 78 (29.5%) 21 (17.2%) 22.8 (18.7–26.9) 
Summary Q = 4.1      23.6 (16.4–30.1) 
         
Active controls-static intervention 74a Community 594 440 162 (27.3%) 121 (27.5%) −0.2 (−5.7–5.3) 
 86a Community 90 63 59 (65.9%) 35 (55.2%) 10.7 (−15.7–26.4) 
 86a Community 44 63 25 (57.1%) 35 (55.2%) 1.9 (−19.1–21.0) 
 87a Community 401 401 201 (50%) 225 (56%) −6 (−12.9–0.9) 
 89a Community 68 65 31 (45.3%) 22 (33.8%) 11.5 (−5.0–28.0) 
         
Summary Q = 6.3      0.4 (−5.4–6.2) 
Active controls-interactive intervention 90a Community 870 961 567 (65.2%) 608 (63.3%) 1.9 (−2.5–6.3) 
 67a Community 202 198 57 (28%) 23 (12%) 16 (8.3–23.7) 
 68a Community 264 424 212 (80.3%) 316 (74.5%) 5.8 (−1.0–12.6) 
 70a Community 132 131 37 (28%) 20 (15%) 13 (3.2–22.8) 
 97a Community 115 122 21 (18%) 16 (13%) 5.0 (−4.2–14.2) 
Summary Q = 13.0      7.9 (2.3–13.5) 
Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
Usual care controls 75a Community 74 78 53 (72%) 48 (62%) 10 (−4.9–24.8) 
 75a Community 73 78 64 (87%) 48 (62%) 25 (11.8–38.0) 
 88a Community 141 122 53 (37.6%) 21 (17.2%) 20.4 (10.0–30.8) 
 88a Community 107 122 78 (29.5%) 21 (17.2%) 22.8 (18.7–26.9) 
Summary Q = 4.1      23.6 (16.4–30.1) 
         
Active controls-static intervention 74a Community 594 440 162 (27.3%) 121 (27.5%) −0.2 (−5.7–5.3) 
 86a Community 90 63 59 (65.9%) 35 (55.2%) 10.7 (−15.7–26.4) 
 86a Community 44 63 25 (57.1%) 35 (55.2%) 1.9 (−19.1–21.0) 
 87a Community 401 401 201 (50%) 225 (56%) −6 (−12.9–0.9) 
 89a Community 68 65 31 (45.3%) 22 (33.8%) 11.5 (−5.0–28.0) 
         
Summary Q = 6.3      0.4 (−5.4–6.2) 
Active controls-interactive intervention 90a Community 870 961 567 (65.2%) 608 (63.3%) 1.9 (−2.5–6.3) 
 67a Community 202 198 57 (28%) 23 (12%) 16 (8.3–23.7) 
 68a Community 264 424 212 (80.3%) 316 (74.5%) 5.8 (−1.0–12.6) 
 70a Community 132 131 37 (28%) 20 (15%) 13 (3.2–22.8) 
 97a Community 115 122 21 (18%) 16 (13%) 5.0 (−4.2–14.2) 
Summary Q = 13.0      7.9 (2.3–13.5) 
a

Random control group.

Table 5

Patient-targeted sociological interventions

Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
78a Community 370 242 144 (39%) 73 (30%) (1.4–16.6) 
79a Community 289 302 142 (49%) 103 (34%) 15 (7.1–22.7) 
80a Community 223 237 85 (38%) 37 (16%) 22 (14.1–29.9) 
91b, d Community Pre, 450c Pre, 473 Pre, 21.4% Pre, 24.1% −3.5 (−7.4–0.4) 
  Post, 450 Post, 473 Post, 38.1% Post, 43.3%   
82a Community 80 94 47 (58.7%) 45 (47.9%) 10.9 (−3.9–25.7) 
83a Community 165 173 64 (39%) 33 (19%) 20.0 (10.5–29.5) 
77a University 151 147 44 (29%) 31 (21%) 8.0 (−1.8–17.8) 
84a Community 772 711 295 (37%) 357 (42%) −5.0 (−9.9–−0.1) 
85a Community 56 57 32 (56.4%) 25 (43.6%) 12.8 (−5.5–31.1) 
Summary Q = 19.8     12.6 (7.4–17.9) 
Reference no.Study settingSample sizePercentage of women screenedEffect95% CI
InterventionControlInterventionControl
78a Community 370 242 144 (39%) 73 (30%) (1.4–16.6) 
79a Community 289 302 142 (49%) 103 (34%) 15 (7.1–22.7) 
80a Community 223 237 85 (38%) 37 (16%) 22 (14.1–29.9) 
91b, d Community Pre, 450c Pre, 473 Pre, 21.4% Pre, 24.1% −3.5 (−7.4–0.4) 
  Post, 450 Post, 473 Post, 38.1% Post, 43.3%   
82a Community 80 94 47 (58.7%) 45 (47.9%) 10.9 (−3.9–25.7) 
83a Community 165 173 64 (39%) 33 (19%) 20.0 (10.5–29.5) 
77a University 151 147 44 (29%) 31 (21%) 8.0 (−1.8–17.8) 
84a Community 772 711 295 (37%) 357 (42%) −5.0 (−9.9–−0.1) 
85a Community 56 57 32 (56.4%) 25 (43.6%) 12.8 (−5.5–31.1) 
Summary Q = 19.8     12.6 (7.4–17.9) 
a

Random control group.

b

Concurrent control group.

c

Pre, preintervention; Post, postintervention.

d

Excluded from quantitative analysis.

1
Shapiro S. Periodic screening for breast cancer: the HIP randomized controlled trial.
J. Natl. Cancer Inst. Monogr.
,
22
:
27
-30,  
1997
.
2
Tabar L., Fagerberg C. J., Duffy S., Day N., Gas A., Grontoft O. Update of the Swedish two-county program of mammographic screening trial.
Radiol. Clin. North Am.
,
30
:
187
-210,  
1992
.
3
Andersson I., Aspegren K., Janzon L., Landberg T., Lindholm K., Linell F., Ljungberg O., Ranstam J., Sigfusson B. Mammographic screening and mortality from breast cancer: the Malmo mammographic screening trial.
Br. Med. J.
,
297
:
943
-948,  
1988
.
4
Hayward R., Shapiro M., Freeman H., Corey C. Who gets screened for cervical and breast cancer?.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
148
:
1177
-1181,  
1988
.
5
Weisman C., Celentano D., Teitelbaum M., Klassen A. Cancer screening services for the elderly.
Public Health Rep.
,
104
:
209
-214,  
1989
.
6
Mandelblatt J., Traxler M., Larkin P., Kanetsky P., Kao R. Targeting breast cancer and cervical cancer screening to elderly poor black women: who will participate?.
Prev. Med.
,
22
:
20
-33,  
1993
.
7
McCool W. F. Barriers to breast cancer screening in older women.
J. Nurse Midwifery
,
39
:
283
-299,  
1994
.
8
Weinberger M. W., Saunders A. F., Samsa G. P., Bearon L. B., Gold D. T., Brown J. T., Booher P., Loehrer P. J. Breast cancer screening in older women: practices and barriers reported by primary care physicians.
J. Am. Geriatr. Soc.
,
39
:
22
-29,  
1991
.
9
Coll P. P., O’Connor P. J., Crabtree B. F., Besdine R. W. Effects of age, education, and physician advice on utilization of screening mammography.
J. Am. Geriatr. Soc.
,
37
:
957
-962,  
1989
.
10
Potosky A. L., Breen N., Graubard B. I., Parsons P. E. The association between health care coverage and the use of cancer screening tests: results from the 1992 National Health Interview Survey.
Med. Care (Phila.)
,
36
:
257
-270,  
1998
.
11
Blustein J. Medicare coverage, supplemental insurance, and the use of mammography by older women.
N. Engl. J. Med.
,
332
:
1138
-1143,  
1995
.
12
Fox S. A., Stein J. A. The effect of physician-patient communication on mammography utilization by different ethnic groups.
Med. Care (Phila.)
,
29
:
1065
-1082,  
1991
.
13
Fox S. A., Siu A. L., Stein J. A. The importance of physician communication on breast cancer screening of older women.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
154
:
2058
-2068,  
1994
.
14
Howard J. Using mammography for cancer control: an unrealized potential.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
37
:
33
-48,  
1987
.
15
O’Malley A. S., Mandelblatt J., Gold K., Cagney K. A., Kerner J. Continuity of care and the use of breast and cervical cancer screening services in a multiethnic community.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
157
:
1462
-1470,  
1997
.
16
Stein J. A., Fox S. A., Murata P. J. The influence of ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and psychological barriers on use of mammography.
J. Health Soc. Behav.
,
32
:
101
-113,  
1991
.
17
Anderson L. M., May D. S. Has the use of cervical, breast, and colorectal cancer screening increased in the United States?.
Am. J. Public Health
,
85
:
840
-842,  
1995
.
18
Woolhandler S., Himmelstein D. U. Reverse targeting of preventive care due to lack of health insurance.
J. Am. Med. Assoc.
,
259
:
2872
-2874,  
1988
.
19
Roberts M. M., Alexander F. E., Elton R. A., Rodgers A. Breast cancer stage, social class, and the impact of screening.
Eur. J. Surg. Oncol.
,
16
:
18
-21,  
1990
.
20
Breen N., Brown M. L. The price of mammography in the United States: data from the National Survey of Mammography Facilities.
Milbank Q.
,
72
:
431
-450,  
1994
.
21
Costanza M. E. The extent of breast cancer screening in older women.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
74
:
2046
-2050,  
1994
.
22
Rimer B. K., Keintz M. K., Kessler H. B., Engstrom P. F., Rosan J. R. Why women resist screening mammography: patient-related barriers.
Radiology
,
172
:
243
-246,  
1989
.
23
Oakar M. R. Legislative effect of the 102nd Congress. Cancer prevention, detection, treatment, and research.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
69
:
1954
-1956,  
1992
.
24
Fletcher S. W., Harris R. P., Gonzalez J. J., Degnan D., Lannin D. R., Strecher V. J., Pilgrim C., Quade D., Earp J. A., Clark R. L. Increasing mammography utilization: a controlled study.
J. Natl. Cancer Inst.
,
85
:
112
-120,  
1993
.
25
Hiatt R. A., Pasick R. J. Unsolved problems in early breast cancer detection: focus on the underserved.
Breast Cancer Res. Treat.
,
40
:
37
-51,  
1996
.
26
Michielutte R., Bahnson J., Beal P. Readability of the public education literature on cancer prevention and decision.
J. Cancer Educ.
,
19
:
51
-55,  
1990
.
27
Davis T. C., Arnold C., Berkel H. J., Nandy I., Jackson R. H., Glass J. Knowledge and attitude on screening mammography among low-literate, low-income women.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
78
:
1912
-1920,  
1996
.
28
Grady K. E., Lemkau J. P., McVay J. M., Reisine S. T. The importance of physician encouragement in breast cancer screening of older women.
Prev. Med.
,
21
:
766
-780,  
1992
.
29
Vernon S. W., Vogel V. G., Halabi S. Factors associated with perceived risk of breast cancer among women attending a screening program.
Breast Cancer Res. Treat.
,
28
:
137
-144,  
1993
.
30
Myers R. E., Ross E. A., Wolf T. A., Balshem A., Jepson C., Millner L. Behavioral interventions to increase adherence in colorectal cancer screening.
Med. Care (Phila.)
,
29
:
1039
-1050,  
1991
.
31
Kagawa-Singer M. Addressing issues for early detection and screening in ethnic populations.
Oncol. Nurs. Forum
,
24
:
1705
-1711,  
1997
.
32
Freeman H. Race, poverty, and cancer.
J. Natl. Cancer Inst.
,
83
:
526
-527,  
1991
.
33
Kreher N. E., Hickner J. M., Ruffin M. T., Lin C. S. Effect of distance and travel on rural women’s compliance with screening mammography: an UPRNet Study.
J. Fam. Pract.
,
40
:
143
-147,  
1995
.
34
Harris R., Leininger L. Preventive care in rural primary care practice.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
72
:
1113
-1118,  
1993
.
35
Katz S. J., Hofer T. P. Socioeconomic disparities in preventive care persist despite universal coverage. Breast and cervical cancer screening in Ontario and the United States.
J. Am. Med. Assoc.
,
272
:
530
-534,  
1994
.
36
Meissner H. I., Breen N., Coyne C., Legler J. M., Green D. T., Edwards B. K. Breast and cervical cancer screening interventions: an assessment of the literature.
Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. Prev.
,
7
:
951
-956,  
1998
.
37
Breen N., Kessler L. Changes in the use of screening mammography: evidence from the 1987 and 1990 National Health Interview Surveys.
Am. J. Public Health
,
84
:
62
-67,  
1994
.
38
Majeed A., Givin-Wilson R., Smith E. Impact of follow-up letters on non-attenders for breast screening: a general practice based study.
J. Med. Screen.
,
4
:
19
-20,  
1997
.
39
Dorsch M. M., Cheok F., Ingham H. M. The effectiveness of invitations from general practitioners in recruiting women to mammographic screening.
Med. J. Aust.
,
155
:
623
-625,  
1991
.
40
Maurer W. J. Breast cancer screening complacency and compliance.
Wisconsin Med. J.
,
94
:
305
-306,  
1995
.
41
Falshaw M. E., Fenton C., Parsons L., Robson J. Improving the uptake of breast screening: one initiative in east London.
Public Health
,
110
:
305
-306,  
1996
.
42
Hoare T., Thomas C., Biggs A., Booth M., Bradley S., Friedman E. Can the uptake of Asian women be increased? A randomized controlled trial of linkworker intervention.
J. Public Health Med.
,
16
:
179
-185,  
1994
.
43
Sharp D. J., Peters T. J., Bartholomew J., Shaw A. Breast screening: a randomised controlled trial in UK general practice of three interventions designed to increase uptake.
J. Epidemiol. Community Health
,
50
:
72
-76,  
1996
.
44
Clover K., Redman S., Forbes J., Sanson-Fisher R., Callaghan T. Two sequential randomized trials of community participation to recruit women for mammographic screening.
Prev. Med.
,
25
:
126
-134,  
1996
.
45
Williams E. M. I., Vessey M. P. Randomized trial of two strategies offering women mobile screening for breast cancer.
Br. Med. J.
,
299
:
158
-159,  
1989
.
46
Hurley S. F., Jolley D. J., Livingston P. M., Reading D., Cockburn J., Flint-Richer D. Effectiveness, costs, and cost-effectiveness of recruitment strategies for a mammographic screening program to detect breast cancer.
J. Natl. Cancer Inst.
,
84
:
855
-863,  
1992
.
47
Clover K. A., Redman S., Forbes J. F., Sanson-Fisher R. W., Dickinson J. A. Promotion of attendance for mammographic screening through general practice: a randomised trial of two strategies.
Med. J. Aust.
,
156
:
92
-94,  
1992
.
48
Drossaert C. H. C., Boer H., Seydel E. R. Health education to improve repeat participation in the Dutch breast cancer screening programme: evaluation of a leaflet tailored to previous participants.
Patient Educ. Couns.
,
28
:
121
-131,  
1996
.
49
Irwig L., Turnbull D., McMurchie M. A randomized trial of general practitioner-written invitations to encourage attendance at screening mammography.
Community Health Stud.
,
14
:
357
-364,  
1990
.
50
Richardson A., Williams S., Elwood M., Bahr M. Participation breast cancer screening: randomized controlled trials of doctors letters and of telephone reminders.
Aust. J. Public Health
,
18
:
290
-292,  
1994
.
51
Atri J., Falshaw M., Gregg R., Robson J., Omar R. Z., Dixon S. Improving uptake of breast screening in multiethnic populations: a randomized controlled trial using practice reception staff to contact non-attenders.
Br. Med. J.
,
315
:
1356
-1359,  
1997
.
52
Ore L., Hagoel L., Shirfoni G., Rennert G. Compliance with mammography screening in Israeli women: the impact of a pre-scheduled appointment and the letter style.
Isr. J. Med. Sci.
,
33
:
103
-111,  
1997
.
53
Schapira D. V., Kumar N. G., Clark R. A., Yag C. Mammography screening credit card and compliance.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
70
:
509
-512,  
1992
.
54
Mayer J. A., Clapp E. J., Bartholomew S., Offer J. Facility-based inreach strategies to promote annual mammograms.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
10
:
353
-356,  
1994
.
55
Kendall C., Hailey B. J. The relative effectiveness of three reminder letters on making and keeping mammogram appointments.
Behav. Med.
,
19
:
29
-34,  
1993
.
56
Fineberg M. V., Funkhouser A. R., Marks M. Variation in medical practice: a review of the literature Eisenberg J. M. eds. .
Doctor’s Decisions and the Cost of Medical Care
,
:
137
-138,  
1986
.
57
DeSimonian R., Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Controlled Clin.
Trials
,
7
:
177
-188,  
1986
.
58
Lau, J. Meta-Analyst Computer Program, copyright John Lau, 1994.
59
Landis S. E., Hulkower S. D., Pierson S. Enhancing adherence with mammography through patient letters and physician prompts: a pilot study.
North Carolina Med. J.
,
53
:
575
-578,  
1992
.
60
Dickey L. L., Petitti D. A patient-held minirecord to promote adult preventive care.
J. Fam. Pract.
,
34
:
457
-463,  
1992
.
61
Mohler P. J. Enhancing compliance with screening mammography recommendations: a clinical trial in a primary care office.
Fam. Med.
,
27
:
117
-121,  
1995
.
62
Burack R. C., Gimotty P. A., George J., Simon M. S., Dews P., Moncrease A. The effect of patient and physician reminders on use of screening mammography in a health maintenance organization. Results of a randomized controlled trial.
Cancer (Phila.)
,
78
:
1708
-1721,  
1996
.
63
Lantz P. M., Stencil D., Lippert M. T., Beversdorf S., Jaros L., Remington P. L. Breast and cervical cancer screening in a low-income managed care sample: the efficacy of physician letters and phone calls.
Am. J. Public Health
,
85
:
834
-836,  
1995
.
64
Turner R. C., Waivers L. E., O’Brien K. The effect of patient-carried reminder cards on the performance of health maintenance measures.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
150
:
645
-647,  
1990
.
65
Ornstein S. M., Garr D. R., Jenkins R. G., Rust P. F., Arnon A. Computer-generated physician and patient reminders: tools to improve population adherence to selected preventive services.
J. Fam. Pract.
,
32
:
82
-90,  
1991
.
66
Taplin S. H., Anderman C., Grothaus L., Curry S., Montano D. Using physician correspondence and postcard reminders to promote mammography use.
Am. J. Public Health
,
84
:
571
-574,  
1994
.
67
King E. S., Rimer B. K., Seay J., Balshem A., Engstrom P. F. Promoting mammography use through progressive interventions: is it effective?.
Am. J. Public Health
,
84
:
104
-106,  
1994
.
68
Margolis K. L., Menart T. C. A test of two interventions to improve compliance with scheduled mammography appointments.
J. Gen. Intern. Med.
,
11
:
539
-541,  
1996
.
69
Somkin C. P., Hiatt R. A., Hurley L. B., Gruskin E., Ackerson L., Larson P. The effect of patient and provider reminders on mammography and Papanicolaou smear screening in large health maintenance organization.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
157
:
1658
-1664,  
1997
.
70
Davis N. A., Nash E., Bailey C., Lewis M. J., Rimer B. K., Koplan J. P. Evaluation of three methods for improving mammography rates in a managed care plan.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
13
:
298
-302,  
1997
.
71
Wolosin R. J. Effect of appointment scheduling and reminder postcards on adherence to mammography recommendations.
J. Fam. Pract.
,
30
:
542
-547,  
1990
.
72
McPhee S. J., Bird J. A., Jenkins C. N. H., Fordham D. Promoting cancer screening: a randomized, controlled trial of three interventions.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
149
:
1866
-1872,  
1989
.
73
Mayer J. A., Jones J. A., Eckhardt L. E., Haliday J., Bartholomew S., Slymen D. J., Hovell M. F. Evaluation of a worksite mammography program.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
9
:
244
-249,  
1993
.
74
Curry S. J., Taplin S. H., Anderman C., Barlow W. E., McBride C. A randomized trial of the impact of risk assessment and feedback on participation in mammography screening.
Prev. Med.
,
22
:
350
-360,  
1993
.
75
Champion V. L. Strategies to increase mammography utilization.
Med. Care (Phila.)
,
32
:
118
-129,  
1994
.
76
Skinner C. S., Strecher V. J., Hospers H. Physician’s recommendations for mammography. Do tailored messages make a difference?.
Am. J. Public Health
,
84
:
43
-49,  
1994
.
77
Davis T. C., Hetkel H., Arnold C., Nandy I., Jackson R. H., Murphy P. W. Intervention to increase mammography utilization in a public hospital.
J. Gen. Intern. Med.
,
13
:
230
-233,  
1998
.
78
Houts P. S., Wojtkowiak S. L., Simmonds M. A., Weinberg G. B., Heitjan D. F. Using a state cancer registry to increase screening behaviors of sisters and daughters of breast cancer patients.
Am. J. Public Health
,
81
:
386
-388,  
1991
.
79
Calle E. E., Miracle-McMahill H. L., Moss R. E., Heath C. W., Jr. Personal contact from friends to increase mammography usage.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
10
:
361
-366,  
1994
.
80
Janz N. K., Schottenfeld D., Doerr K. M., Selig S. M., Dunn R. L., Strawderman M., Levine P. A. A two-step intervention to increase mammography among women aged 65 and older.
Am. J. Public Health
,
87
:
1683
-1686,  
1997
.
81
Suarez L., Nichols D. C., Brady C. A. Use of peer role models to increase Pap smear and mammography screening in Mexican-American and black women.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
9
:
290
-296,  
1993
.
82
Sung J. F. C., Blumenthal D. S., Coates R. J., Williams J. E., Alema-Mensah E., Liff J. M. Effect of cancer screening intervention conducted by lay health workers among inner-city women.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
13
:
51
-57,  
1997
.
83
Weber B. E., Reilly B. M. Enhanced mammography use in the inner city: a randomized trial of intensive case management.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
157
:
2345
-2349,  
1997
.
84
Margolis K. L., Lurie N., McGovern P. G., Tyrrell M., Slater J. S. Increasing breast and cervical cancer screening in low-income women.
J. Gen. Intern. Med.
,
13
:
515
-521,  
1998
.
85
Navarro A. M., Senn K. L., McNicholas L. J., Kaplan R. M., Roppe B., Campo M. C. Por La Vida model intervention enhances use of cancer screening test among Latinas.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
15
:
32
-41,  
1998
.
86
Rothman A. J., Salovey P., Turvey C., Fishkin S. A. Attributions of responsibility and persuasion: increasing mammography utilization among women over 40 with an internally oriented message.
Health Psychol.
,
12
:
39
-47,  
1993
.
87
Bastani R., Marcus A. C., Maxwell A. E., Das I. P., Yan K. X. Evaluation of an intervention to increase mammography screening in Los Angeles.
Prev. Med.
,
23
:
83
-90,  
1994
.
88
Aiken L. S., West S. G., Woodward C. K., Reno R. R., Reynolds K D. Increasing screening mammography in asymptomatic women: evaluation of a second-generation, theory-based program.
Health Psychol.
,
13
:
526
-538,  
1994
.
89
Banks S. M., Salovey P., Greener S., Rothman A. J., Moyer A., Beauvais J., Epel E. The effects of message framing on mammography utilization.
Health Psychol.
,
14
:
178
-184,  
1995
.
90
Marcus A. C., Bastani R., Reardon K., Karlins S., Das I. P., Van Herle M. P., McClatchey M. W., Crane L. A. Proactive screening mammography counseling within the cancer information service: results from a randomized trial.
J. Natl. Cancer Inst. Monogr.
,
14
:
119
-129,  
1993
.
91
Suarez L., Roche R. A., Pulley L., Weiss N. S., Goldman D., Simpson D. M. Why a peer intervention program for Mexican-American women failed to modify the secular trend in cancer screening.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
13
:
411
-417,  
1997
.
92
Stoner T. J., Dowd B., Carr W. P., Maldonado G., Church T. R., Mandel J. Do vouchers improve breast cancer screening rates? Results from a randomized trial.
Health Serv. Res.
,
33
:
11
-28,  
1998
.
93
Kiefe C. I., McKay S. V., Halevy A., Brody B. A. Is cost a barrier to screening mammography for low-income women receiving Medicare benefits? A randomized trial.
Arch. Intern. Med.
,
154
:
1217
-1224,  
1994
.
94
Rimer B. K., Resch N., King E., Ross E., Lerman C., Boyce A., Kessler H., Engstrom P. F. Multistrategy health education program to increase mammography use among women ages 65 and older.
Public Health Rep.
,
107
:
369
-380,  
1992
.
95
Clementz G. L., Aldag J. C., Gladfelter T. T., Barclay A. M., Brooks H. F. A randomized study of cancer screening in a family practice setting using a recall model.
J. Fam. Pract.
,
30
:
537
-541,  
1990
.
96
Richardson J. L., Mondrus G. T., Danley K., Deapen D., Mack T. Impact of a mailed intervention on annual mammography and physician breast examination among women at high risk of breast cancer.
Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. Prev.
,
5
:
71
-76,  
1996
.
97
King E., Rimer B. K., Benincasa T., Harrop C., Amfoh K., Bonney G., Kornguth P., Demark-Wahnefried W., Strigo T., Engstrom P. Strategies to encourage mammography use among women in senior citizens housing facilities.
J. Cancer Educ.
,
13
:
108
-115,  
1998
.
98
Turner B. J., Day S. C., Borenstein B. A controlled trial to improve delivery of preventive care: physician or patient reminders?.
J. Gen. Intern. Med.
,
4
:
403
-409,  
1989
.
99
Becker M. H., Maiman L. A. Sociobehavioral determinants of compliance with health and medical care recommendations.
Med. Care (Phila.)
,
13
:
10
-24,  
1975
.
100
Wagner T. H. The effectiveness of mailed patient reminders on mammography screenings: a meta-analysis.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
14
:
64
-70,  
1998
.
101
Zapka J. G., Bigelow C., Hurley T., Ford L. D., Egelhofer J., Cloud W. M., Sachsse E. Mammography use among sociodemographically diverse women: the accuracy of self-report.
Am. J. Public Health
,
86
:
1016
-1021,  
1996
.
102
McGovern P. G., Lurie N., Margolis K. L., Slater J. S. Accuracy of self-report of mammography and Pap smear in a low-income urban population.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
14
:
201
-208,  
1998
.
103
Champion V. L., Menon U., McQuillen D. H., Scott C. Validity of self-reported mammography in low-income African-American women.
Am. J. Prev. Med.
,
14
:
111
-117,  
1998
.
104
Elmore J. G., Barton M. B., Moceri V. M., Polk S., Arena P. J., Fletcher S. W. Ten-year risk of false positive screening mammograms and clinical breast exams.
N. Engl. J. Med.
,
338
:
1089
-1096,  
1998
.
105
Lerman C., Trock B., Rimer B. K., Boyce A., Jepson C., Engstrom P. F. Psychological and behavioral implications of abnormal mammograms.
Ann. Intern. Med.
,
114
:
657
-661,  
1991
.