Abstract
Background: We explored racial/ethnic differences in perceived cancer risk and determinants of these differences in a nationally representative sample of whites, blacks, Hispanics, and Asians.
Methods: Multiple regression techniques, including mediational analyses, were used to identify determinants and quantify racial/ethnic differences in the perception of the risk of developing cancer among 5,581 adult respondents to the 2007 Health Information Trends Survey (HINTS).
Results: Blacks, Hispanics, and Asians reported lower perceived cancer risk than whites [Bs = −0.40, −0.34, and −0.69, respectively; (Ps < 0.001)]. Contributing factors included relatively lower likelihood of reporting a family history of cancer, lower likelihood of having smoked, and a less strong belief that everything causes cancer among nonwhites than among whites. Racial/ethnic differences in perceived risk were attenuated in older respondents because perceived cancer risk was negatively associated with age for whites but not for nonwhites.
Conclusions: Nonwhites had lower perceptions of cancer risk than whites. Some of the racial/ethnic variability in perceived risk may be due to racial and ethnic differences in awareness of one's family history of cancer and its relevance for cancer risk, experiences with behavioral risk factors, and salience of cancer risk information. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev; 19(3); 746–54
Perceived Cancer Risk: Why Is It Lower Among Nonwhites than Whites?
Perceived risk for health problems is a key motivator for taking preventive action (1, 2). With respect to cancer, an individual's perceived cancer risk (PCR) predicts her/his likelihood of engaging in preventive health behaviors, including cancer screening (3-5). Some evidence indicates that there may be differences in PCR as a function of race/ethnicity, with nonwhites reporting lower perceived risk relative to whites (6-9). Low PCR may be a barrier to screening and other prevention behaviors, thus contributing to unnecessary cancer morbidity and mortality among nonwhites.
A small number of studies have investigated racial/ethnic variability in cancer risk perceptions yielding mixed results. Black, Hispanic, and other nonwhite respondents to the 2000 National Health Interview Survey had lower global (all sites) PCR than whites (9). Among ∼800 participants in a nocost oral cancer screening in the New York City area, relative to other racial/ethnic groups, Asians reported lower perceived risk for oral cancer (8). In a diverse sample of women recruited from primary care clinics in San Francisco, Asians reported lower perceived risk for cervical, breast, and colon cancer than whites, but Hispanics reported higher perceived risk compared with whites for colon and cervical cancer, and perceptions did not vary between whites and blacks (7). Studies have also shown that blacks are less likely to perceive increased risk due to family history of cancer than whites, or their black counterparts without a family history of cancer (6, 10-12).
Overall, blacks have higher incidence and mortality from cancer (13); therefore, low perception of risk in this population is of particular concern. Although Asians and Hispanics have lower all-site cancer incidence and mortality than whites (13), lower-than-average risk does not equate to no cancer risk. Even in these groups, inappropriately low PCR remains a concern to the extent that it hinders cancer screening or other cancer prevention behaviors. Although not all studies link cancer risk perception and cancer screening, narrative and meta-analytic reviews indicate that, overall, screening is more likely among people with higher perceived risk (3-5). There is ample empirical evidence that nonwhites are less likely than whites to be screened for breast, cervical, colorectal, and prostate cancer. Specifically, mammography, cervical screening, colorectal cancer screening, and prostate cancer screening is lower among Hispanics and Asians than whites (14-16). Blacks and whites are equivalently likely to receive prostate and cervical cancer screening (15, 17); however, blacks may be less likely to receive regular mammograms or colorectal cancer screening (14, 15, 17-19). Given lower screening rates among nonwhites and the importance of risk perception to cancer screening use, research is needed to better understand the construction of cancer risk perceptions in these groups.
The extant studies on racial and ethnic differences in risk perception have three key limitations. First, most are based on small, nonrepresentative samples; therefore, additional studies of large, nationally representative samples are needed to confirm the generalizability of observed race/ethnic differences in PCR. The present study uses data from the National Cancer Institute's 2007 nationally representative Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS). Second, the studies published to date have described differences in risk as a function of race/ethnicity, but have not explored possible factors accounting for those differences. In this study, we explored two classes of explanations for why racial/ethnic differences in PCR may exist. First, it is possible that race/ethnicity is associated with differences in predictors of PCR that, in turn, predict racial/ethnic differences in PCR (mediation hypothesis). For example, nonwhites are less likely to report having a family history of cancer (20-22), which could, in turn, result in lower PCR among nonwhites compared with whites. To examine this possibility, we identified potential determinants of PCR and tested their role as mediators of racial/ethnic differences in PCR. A second possibility is that the strength or direction of the relationship between factors that typically predict PCR differ as a function of race/ethnicity (moderation hypothesis). For example, if factors that one would expect to increase PCR, such as having a family history of cancer, do not do so among nonwhites, PCR may be lower among nonwhites than whites. For example, evidence indicates that blacks with a family history of cancer do not have an increased perception of risk compared with those without a family history of cancer, whereas for whites, family history is a robust predictor of increased PCR (6, 10-12). To test this second explanation, we conducted a series of analyses to test for interactions between race and determinants of PCR. Third, in some previous studies, race/ethnic differences in PCR were not observed (7, 23); however, these previous analyses were done on data from people who were 45 years or older or 50 years or older. In majority white samples, absolute PCR has been found to be lower among older than younger respondents (9). Racial/ethnic differences in PCR may be reduced as a function of age if PCR is relatively lower among older whites but not older nonwhites. In the present work, we examined whether this is the case in an attempt to resolve inconsistencies in the literature on racial/ethnic differences in PCR.
Materials and Methods
Sample
Data for the study were obtained from the 2007 HINTS, a probability-based survey conducted by the National Cancer Institute to study cancer health information seeking cancer knowledge and health behavior. The survey used a dual-frame design [random digit dial (RDD) and mail] due to concerns about decreasing response rates and lack of coverage for cell phone–only households when RDD is used alone. Response rates for the RDD sample were 42.37% for the screener interview and 57.19% for the full interview, yielding an overall response rate of 24.23% (n = 4,081; ref. 24). The overall response rate for the mail sample was 30.99% (n = 3,593). Survey respondents were excluded if they did not self-identify as one of the four races/ethnicities of interest (white, black, Hispanic, or Asian; n = 718), if they reported having been diagnosed with cancer in the past (n = 942), if they did not respond to the PCR item (n = 222), or if they had missing data on other key variables of interest (gender, marital status, education, family history of cancer, smoking status, and perceived health; n = 211). Analyses were done on a final sample of 5,581 adult respondents (mail, 2,768; RDD, 2,820; white, 4,319; black, 567; Hispanic, 514; Asian, 181). On average, respondents who were dropped from the analyses due to missing data on PCR or the other variables of interest (433) were older (t = 2.83; P = 0.006), less educated (t = −3.58; P = 0.001), less likely to be married (χ2 = 10.12; P = 0.002); had worse perceived health (t = −2.43; P = 0.02), were more likely to be black (χ2 = 7.08; P = 0.009), or Hispanic (χ2 = 11.02; P = 0.001) than white, and perceived their cancer risk to be lower (t = −2.32; P = 0.02) than respondents whose data were included the study. The groups did not vary with respect to likelihood of reporting a family history of cancer, smoking status, or the belief that everything causes cancer (Ps > 0.15).
Variables
Demographic Characteristics
Participants self-classified as non-Hispanic white, Hispanic, non-Hispanic black or African-American, or non-Hispanic Asian. The four groups are hereafter called white, black, Hispanic, and Asian. Gender, age, marital status (married or living with a partner versus divorced, widowed, separated, or never been married), and education (less than high school, high school, some college, college, or greater) were assessed through self-report.
Perceived Cancer Risk
The outcome of interest was the perception of one's overall risk for developing cancer or PCR. Respondents were asked to indicate their level of perceived risk on a scale of absolute risk: “How likely do you think it is that you will develop cancer in the future? Would you say your chance of getting cancer is very low/somewhat low/moderate/somewhat high/very high?” Responses were converted to a five-point response format with higher scores representing greater PCR.
Potential Mediators/Moderators
Several factors that have been associated with PCR were examined (reporting a family history of cancer, having been a cigarette smoker in the past or present, perceived health, and beliefs about cancer; refs. 3, 5, 9, 25). Participants were asked “Have any of your family members ever had cancer?” Respondents were classified as having had one or more family members with cancer or no family members with cancer. Respondents were classified as ever smokers if they had smoked 100 or more cigarettes in their lifetime or never smokers if they had not. Perceived health was assessed by asking respondents “in general, would you say your health is excellent/very good/good/fair/poor?” Responses were recoded so that higher responses represented better health. Beliefs about cancer seriousness, early detection, and prevention were assessed with single item measures and four-point response formats, ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree, and were reverse coded so that higher responses represented greater agreement. These items were (a) “When I think of cancer, I automatically think of death”; (b) “Cancer is most often caused by a person's behavior or life-style”; (c) “Getting checked regularly for cancer helps find cancer when it's easy to treat”; (d) “Cancer is an illness that when detected early can typically be cured”; (e) “It seems like everything causes cancer”; (f) “There's not much you can do to lower your chances of getting cancer”; and (g) “There are so many different recommendations about preventing cancer, it's hard to know which ones to follow.” Responses to these individual items were typically not correlated (mean r = 0.11; range = 0.00-0.33); therefore, these items were analyzed individually. An additional cancer belief item asked people “Overall, how many people who develop cancer do you think survive at least 5 years (less than 25%/about 25%/about 50%/about 75%/nearly all)?” Responses were converted to a five-point response format with higher scores representing greater perceived likelihood of survival.
Data Analysis
All analyses used data that were weighted to produce nationally representative estimates. Simple logistic and linear regressions were used to assess differences in the categorical and continuous variables as a function of race. Multiple linear regression was used to assess the adjusted effects of hypothesized predictors on PCR. We identified the explanatory factors for the hypothesized racial/ethnic differences in PCR using mediation analysis techniques described by Baron and Kenny (26).5
5All analyses included sample mode as a covariate. Participants in the final sample of mail respondents had higher education than those in the final sample of RDD respondents (Ps <.05). In addition, after controlling for other variables included in the multivariate analyses (age, marital status, education, gender, family history of cancer, smoking status), PCR varied as a function of an interaction between race/ethnicity and mode (−0.43, −0.73 to −0.10). This was attributable to the fact that Asians in the RDD sample reported lower PCR [M, 1.85; 95% confidence interval (95% CI), 1.53-2.17] than Asians in the mail sample (M, 2.33; 95% CI, 2.11-2.56). It should be noted, however, that the Asian subsamples were small (119 in the mail sample and 62 in the RDD sample).There were no other significant interaction effects between sample mode and the variables included in the analyses on PCR (all Ps > 0.2).
6We doubt that not including income as a covariate significantly affected the results of the analyses. Income was not associated with PCR. In addition, we included education that covaried with income (r = 0.44, P < 0.001).
Results
Sample Characteristics and Predictors of PCR
Characteristics of the total sample and of each race/ethnicity group are presented in Table 1. There were several statistically significant group differences (P < 0.05). Hispanic respondents were more likely to be male than white respondents; black respondents were less likely to be married than white respondents; blacks and Hispanics had less education than whites and Asians had more education than whites; and respondents from the three nonwhite groups were younger, on average, than white respondents. Blacks, Hispanics, and Asians were significantly less likely than whites to report a family history of cancer; Hispanics and Asians were less likely to have smoked cigarettes than whites; blacks and Hispanics perceived that they were less healthy than whites.
. | Total sample % . | White % . | Black % . | Hispanic % . | Asian % . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Female | 50.69 (49.94-51.44) | 51.23 (50.32-52.15) | 57.25 (52.14-62.37) | 45.17* (40.65-49.69) | 42.16 (32.00-52.32) |
Married | 57.20 (56.35-58.06) | 60.31 (59.46-61.52) | 36.32† (32.19-40.45) | 57.58 (52.49-62.67) | 61.08 (49.58-72.57) |
College or greater | 24.91 (24.39-25.44) | 27.64 (26.96-28.32) | 16.40† (13.16-19.64) | 10.23† (7.61-12.84) | 45.96† (35.73-56.19) |
Family history of cancer | 70.10 (68.14-72.07) | 76.67 (74.55-78.80) | 64.86† (59.13-70.59) | 51.91† (44.79-59.03) | 36.71† (24.43-49.00) |
Ever smoker | 45.65 (43.60-47.71) | 48.68 (46.51-50.86) | 44.50 (38.55-50.45) | 39.99‡ (33.59-46.39) | 19.66† (10.97-28.35) |
Total sample mean | White mean | Black mean | Hispanic mean | Asian mean | |
Age | 44.33 (44.33-44.60) | 46.27 (45.95-46.59) | 43.85‡ (42.07-45.63) | 36.50† (35.01-37.92) | 38.73† (35.35-42.12) |
Perceived health (of 5) | 3.42 (3.38-3.56) | 3.48 (3.43-3.53) | 3.32‡ (3.21-3.43) | 3.22† (3.09-3.35) | 3.30 (3.07-3.54) |
PCR (of 5) | 2.62 (2.58-2.67) | 2.73 (2.69-2.78) | 2.34† (2.22-2.46) | 2.46† (2.32-2.59) | 2.09† (1.90-2.29) |
. | Total sample % . | White % . | Black % . | Hispanic % . | Asian % . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Female | 50.69 (49.94-51.44) | 51.23 (50.32-52.15) | 57.25 (52.14-62.37) | 45.17* (40.65-49.69) | 42.16 (32.00-52.32) |
Married | 57.20 (56.35-58.06) | 60.31 (59.46-61.52) | 36.32† (32.19-40.45) | 57.58 (52.49-62.67) | 61.08 (49.58-72.57) |
College or greater | 24.91 (24.39-25.44) | 27.64 (26.96-28.32) | 16.40† (13.16-19.64) | 10.23† (7.61-12.84) | 45.96† (35.73-56.19) |
Family history of cancer | 70.10 (68.14-72.07) | 76.67 (74.55-78.80) | 64.86† (59.13-70.59) | 51.91† (44.79-59.03) | 36.71† (24.43-49.00) |
Ever smoker | 45.65 (43.60-47.71) | 48.68 (46.51-50.86) | 44.50 (38.55-50.45) | 39.99‡ (33.59-46.39) | 19.66† (10.97-28.35) |
Total sample mean | White mean | Black mean | Hispanic mean | Asian mean | |
Age | 44.33 (44.33-44.60) | 46.27 (45.95-46.59) | 43.85‡ (42.07-45.63) | 36.50† (35.01-37.92) | 38.73† (35.35-42.12) |
Perceived health (of 5) | 3.42 (3.38-3.56) | 3.48 (3.43-3.53) | 3.32‡ (3.21-3.43) | 3.22† (3.09-3.35) | 3.30 (3.07-3.54) |
PCR (of 5) | 2.62 (2.58-2.67) | 2.73 (2.69-2.78) | 2.34† (2.22-2.46) | 2.46† (2.32-2.59) | 2.09† (1.90-2.29) |
*Subgroup significantly different from whites (P < 0.01).
†Subgroup significantly different from whites (P < 0.001).
‡Subgroup significantly different from whites (P < 0.05).
Racial Differences in PCR
On average, respondents in each of the nonwhite groups had lower PCR than whites (Table 1). After adjusting for demographic characteristics (age, gender, education, and marital status) and sample mode (mail versus RDD; ref. 3), being black (B = −0.40; 95% CI, 0.53 to −0.27), Hispanic (B = −0.34; 95% CI, −0.48 to -0.19), or Asian (B = −0.69; 95% CI, −0.90 to −0.48) compared with white still predicted lower PCR.
Mediators of Racial Differences in PCR
Results of the mediational analyses to explore whether racial/ethnic differences in determinants of PCR explained racial/ethnic differences in PCR are presented in Table 2. As can be seen in the table, the race-PCR relation was mediated by family history, smoking status, and the strength of one's belief that everything causes cancer. After the adjustment for other demographic characteristics, 14.6% of the black-white difference in PCR was explained by less frequent reporting of family history of cancer among blacks than whites. Part of the Hispanic-white difference in PCR (29.8%) was explained by relatively less frequent reporting of a family history of cancer among Hispanics than whites, and 10.4% of the difference by lower likelihood of having been or being a smoker among Hispanics than among whites. Twenty-eight percent of the Asian-white difference in PCR was explained by less frequent reporting of a family history of cancer by Asians than whites and 13.4% by lower likelihood of having been a smoker among Asians than among whites. All of the above mediating (indirect) effects were statistically significant (see the results of the Sobel tests presented in Table 2). Table 2 also provides the relevant coefficients from the regression models used to test for mediation.
. | Race → PCR . | Race → mediator . | Mediator → PCR controlling for race . | Race → PCR controlling for mediator . | Sobel statistic (P) . | % Effect mediated . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hypothesized mediator = family history of cancer | ||||||
Black | −0.41 (−0.54 to −0.28) | −0.62 (−0.90 to −0.34) | 0.48 (0.39-0.57) | −0.35 (−0.48 to −0.22) | −3.99 (<0.001) | 14.6 |
Hispanic | −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) | −1.06 (−1.39 to −0.73) | 0.44 (0.35-0.53) | −0.24 (−0.39 to −0.10) | −5.29 (<0.001) | 29.8 |
Asian | −0.71 (−0.92 to −0.50) | −1.79 (−2.36 to −1.22) | 0.48 (0.38-0.57) | −0.52 (−0.73 to −0.30) | −5.34 (<0.00) | 27.2 |
Hypothesized mediator = smoking status | ||||||
Black | −0.41 (−0.54 to −0.27) | Ns | ||||
Hispanic | −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) | −0.43 (−0.72 to −0.14) | 0.35 (0.26-0.44) | −0.31 (−0.45 to −0.17) | −2.77 (0.006) | 10.4 |
Asian | −0.71 (−0.92 to −0.50) | −1.23 (−1.79 to −0.68) | 0.38 (0.30-0.46) | −0.61 (−0.82 to −0.41) | −4.02 (<0.001) | 13.4 |
Hypothesized mediator = belief that everything causes cancer | ||||||
Black | −0.41 (−0.54 to −0.27) | −0.28 (−0.45 to −0.11) | 0.15 (0.11-0.19) | −0.37 (−0.50 to −0.24) | −3.03 (0.002) | 8.2 |
Hispanic | −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) | −0.38 (−0.55 to −0.21) | 0.15 (0.11-0.20) | −0.28 (−0.43 to −0.14) | −3.66 (0.002) | 17.9 |
Asian | −0.71 (−0.92 to −0.50) | −0.39 (−0.64 to −0.15) | 0.18 (0.14-0.23) | −0.62 (−0.81 to −0.43) | −3.15 (0.002) | 12.3 |
. | Race → PCR . | Race → mediator . | Mediator → PCR controlling for race . | Race → PCR controlling for mediator . | Sobel statistic (P) . | % Effect mediated . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hypothesized mediator = family history of cancer | ||||||
Black | −0.41 (−0.54 to −0.28) | −0.62 (−0.90 to −0.34) | 0.48 (0.39-0.57) | −0.35 (−0.48 to −0.22) | −3.99 (<0.001) | 14.6 |
Hispanic | −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) | −1.06 (−1.39 to −0.73) | 0.44 (0.35-0.53) | −0.24 (−0.39 to −0.10) | −5.29 (<0.001) | 29.8 |
Asian | −0.71 (−0.92 to −0.50) | −1.79 (−2.36 to −1.22) | 0.48 (0.38-0.57) | −0.52 (−0.73 to −0.30) | −5.34 (<0.00) | 27.2 |
Hypothesized mediator = smoking status | ||||||
Black | −0.41 (−0.54 to −0.27) | Ns | ||||
Hispanic | −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) | −0.43 (−0.72 to −0.14) | 0.35 (0.26-0.44) | −0.31 (−0.45 to −0.17) | −2.77 (0.006) | 10.4 |
Asian | −0.71 (−0.92 to −0.50) | −1.23 (−1.79 to −0.68) | 0.38 (0.30-0.46) | −0.61 (−0.82 to −0.41) | −4.02 (<0.001) | 13.4 |
Hypothesized mediator = belief that everything causes cancer | ||||||
Black | −0.41 (−0.54 to −0.27) | −0.28 (−0.45 to −0.11) | 0.15 (0.11-0.19) | −0.37 (−0.50 to −0.24) | −3.03 (0.002) | 8.2 |
Hispanic | −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) | −0.38 (−0.55 to −0.21) | 0.15 (0.11-0.20) | −0.28 (−0.43 to −0.14) | −3.66 (0.002) | 17.9 |
Asian | −0.71 (−0.92 to −0.50) | −0.39 (−0.64 to −0.15) | 0.18 (0.14-0.23) | −0.62 (−0.81 to −0.43) | −3.15 (0.002) | 12.3 |
NOTE: Untransformed coefficients and 95% CIs for pathways tested to determine mediation and results of the Sobel Test of statistical significance for the mediating effects. All models included age, marital status, gender, and education as covariates.
The associations between beliefs about cancer seriousness, early detection and prevention, and PCR were very modest (|B|s = 0.04-0.17); only one of the cancer beliefs, “it seems like everything causes cancer,” partially mediated race/ethnicity differences in PCR (see Table 2). After adjusting for demographic characteristics, less strong belief that everything causes cancer among Hispanics compared with whites accounted for 17.9% of the Hispanic-white difference in PCR. Similarly, less strong belief that everything causes cancer among Asians accounted for 12.3% of the difference in the Asian-white difference in PCR. Strength of belief that everything causes cancer did not differ between blacks and whites; therefore, we did not test for mediation. Perceived health was associated with race but it did not mediate the effects of race/ethnicity on PCR.
Interactions between Race/Ethnicity and Predictors of PCR
Racial/ethnic differences in PCR could also arise if typical determinants of PCR influence PCR among whites but not among nonwhites. For this to be the case, one would expect that an interaction between race/ethnicity and a given determinant predicts PCR, and for the determinant to influence PCR (e.g., family history status) among whites but not among nonwhites (or to a lesser degree, among nonwhites). Interactions between race and smoking status and race and perceived health were not significant (Ps > 0.33). There was a marginally significant interaction between race/ethnicity (Hispanic versus white) and family history (B = −0.26; 95% CI, −0.56 to 0.04). It was due to the fact that, among whites, reporting a family history of cancer was positively associated with higher PCR (B = 0.49; 95% CI, 0.40-0.59), but among Hispanics, reporting a family history of cancer and PCR were not significantly associated with one another (B = 0.19; 95% CI, −0.10 to 0.49). Of note, among blacks, PCR was positively associated with reporting a family history of cancer (B = 0.43; 95% CI, 0.14-0.71). The relationship was weaker and did not reach significance for Asians (B = 0.29; 95% CI, −0.11 to −0.69).
We explored whether race interacted with any of the cancer beliefs to predict PCR. The association between the belief that “everything causes cancer” and PCR varied by race/ethnicity [black versus white (B = −0.14; 95% CI, −0.27 to −0.01); Hispanic versus white (B = −0.12; 95% CI, −0.23 to −0.02)]. These interaction effects were due to the fact that among whites, belief that everything causes cancer was positively associated with PCR (B = 0.17; 95% CI, 0.13-0.22), but no such association was found for blacks (B = 0.03; 95% CI, −0.08 to 0.15) or Hispanics (B = 0.05; 95% CI, −0.06 to 0.16). In addition, the effect of believing that cancer is caused by behavior or life-style on PCR varied by race/ethnicity (Asian versus white; B = 0.30; 95% CI, 0.06-0.58). This interaction was attributable to the fact that in the Asian group, respondents perceived themselves at greater risk to the extent that they believed that cancer was caused by behavior or life-style (B = 0.27; 95% CI, 0.04-0.49), but whites did not (B = 0.03; 95% CI, −0.02 to 0.09). None of the other beliefs significantly interacted with race to predict PCR (Ps > 0.13).
Cancer Risk Perceptions across the Life span
On average, being older was associated with lower PCR; however, this main effect masked racial/ethnic differences in the slopes for PCR as a function of age. Figure 1 displays mean trajectories for PCR as a function of race. Slopes differed significantly between Hispanics and whites (B = 0.009; 95% CI, 0.001-0.018) and between Asians and whites (B = 0.02; 95% CI, 0.009-0.039). The black versus white difference did not reach significance (B = 0.006; 95% CI, −0.002 to.014). Follow-up analysis revealed that for whites, being older was associated with comparatively lower PCR (B = −0.008; 95% CI, −0.011 to −0.006), but that differences as a function of age did not reach statistical significance for any of the nonwhite groups (Ps > 0.52).
Discussion
Black, Hispanic, and Asian respondents to HINTS perceived themselves at lower risk for getting cancer (lower PCR) than white respondents. These differences remained after controlling for age, education, marital status, gender, and the mode in which the survey was administered (mail versus RDD). Blacks perceived their cancer risk to be lower than whites, in part, because they were less likely to have or know that they have a family history of cancer, and because they were less likely to believe that everything causes cancer than whites. Similar patterns were found for Hispanics and Asians, except that being less likely to have smoked also accounted for lower PCR among Hispanics and Asians with respect to whites. Although adding these mediators to the adjusted model of PCR significantly reduced the association between race and PCR, even after accounting for these effects, being black, Hispanic, or Asian remained significantly associated with lower PCR. Additional research is needed to identify other factors that contribute to racial/ethnic differences in PCR.
The relation between race and reported family history of cancer is consistent with past research (20), including a previous study in which we found that nonwhites, and especially immigrants within these groups, were less likely to report a family history of cancer than whites and nonimmigrants (22). Given disparities in cancer incidence and prevalence, blacks should have a greater likelihood of reporting that they have or had a family member who was diagnosed with cancer than whites. Although other Asians and Hispanics have lower cancer incidence, in our previous work, we suggested that the differences in family history of cancer reporting were in excess of what would be expected due to racial/ethnic differences in cancer incidence (22), and that these differences are likely due, in part, to nonwhites and immigrants being relatively less likely to know their family history of cancer. We predicted that less knowledge of family history of cancer among immigrants and racial/ethnic minorities could result in health care providers and nonwhites themselves, underestimating their cancer risk. Results from the present study support this hypothesis. Lower reporting of family history of cancer among nonwhites, compared with whites, did predict relatively lower PCR in nonwhites.
Hispanics and Asians were less likely than whites to have smoked during their lifetimes, which, in turn, was associated with lower PCR compared with whites. It is realistic for people who have never smoked to appraise their cancer risk as lower than those who have; however, future research should investigate whether people distinguish between smoking as a risk factor for cancers that are strongly associated with smoking (e.g., lung cancer, bladder cancer) and others that are less strongly associated with smoking (e.g., breast cancer, prostate cancer). It is possible that generalized beliefs that, as a nonsmoker, one is at relatively lower risk for all cancers could impede appropriate screening and other prevention behaviors. Physical inactivity and low fruit and vegetable consumption are associated with increased risk for several kinds of cancer (29). According to the 2005 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System survey, there are racial/ethnic differences in physical activity and fruit and vegetable consumption (30). However, self-reported physical activity is higher among whites than among blacks, Hispanics, or Asian/Pacific Islanders, a pattern that would not account for lower perceptions of risk among nonwhites compared with whites. Asians/Pacific Islanders consume more fruits and vegetables than whites (no differences across the other three races). Fruit and vegetable consumption was not assessed in the full HINTS 2007 sample; therefore, we were not able to explore whether fruit and vegetable consumption accounted for lower PCR among Asians compared with whites.
We tested a second model of racial/ethnic differences in PCR: whether factors that typically predict PCR only do so for whites, contributing to the lower PCR among nonwhites with respect to whites. For example, if nonwhite groups have had less access to personally relevant cancer risk information, do not believe that cancer risk information is personally applicable, or do not trust cancer risk information (31, 32), their PCR may not be calibrated to known risk factors (e.g., family history of cancer, smoking). This explanation received some support. Among Hispanics, PCR varied less strongly as a function of family history of cancer than among whites and blacks. Hispanics may be less aware of family history risk due to language and other barriers that can make the dissemination of health information difficult. The finding is important for qualifying previous reports that people with a family history of cancer perceive themselves at higher risk for cancer (3, 9, 23): this may not be true among Hispanics. In combination, lower reporting (and knowledge) of family history of cancer and lack of influence of family history on PCR among Hispanics suggest that tailored public health and health care provider–delivered messages about the importance of family history for cancer risk and encouraging family communication about cancer may be a means of increasing awareness of cancer risk among nonwhites.
Race Differences in PCR across the Life span
The findings are consistent with and expand on previous research that showed that older age is associated with lower PCR (9) by showing that the effect varies as a function of race. PCR was lower in older whites than younger whites, but similar downward slopes were not found among nonwhites. This, in effect, narrowed the gap between PCR in older whites and nonwhites. Racial/ethnic differences in age-related trajectories would explain why one might not find racial/ethnic differences in PCR in samples populated primarily by older adults; this would not be due to high PCR among older nonwhites, but rather to low PCR among older whites. The age-related differences in PCR among whites could be a cohort effect if large-scale cancer prevention awareness efforts initiated in the early 1980s (33) only reached certain sectors of the public. Efforts to communicate about cancer and cancer risk and prevention may have been more successful at reaching whites coming into adulthood during this period, but less successful at reaching nonwhites and whites who were older at the time.
Do Individual-Level Cancer Risk Perceptions Reflect Community-Level Cancer Salience?
The existence of racial/ethnic differences in PCR suggests that community-level dynamics influence PCR. Individual-level perceptions of cancer risk may vary as a function of the salience of the disease in one's larger racial/ethnic community and/or peer groups. In the United States, social networks tend to be racially homogenous (34) and race/ethnicity is a central component of identity and social categorization (35); therefore, people may be most likely to base their cancer risk perceptions on the experiences and beliefs of same-race/ethnic peers and models, and images of members of their race/ethnic group. Cancer risk salience may reflect cancer incidence and mortality in one's racial/ethnic group, prevalence of cancer in one's social network, and the degree to which cancer is discussed openly within one's network and larger community. Cultural variability in openness about cancer (36-39) may also contribute to the relative salience of cancer risk in a given community. Risk perceptions, and even whether risk perception is a meaningful construct for a given group, are shaped by culturally informed systems of beliefs about health and illness (40). These systems of beliefs may be inconsistent with those promoted by the mainstream U.S. medical establishment, which could reduce the relevance and effectiveness of traditional risk and prevention communication in some communities (40).
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions
There has been limited research on variability in cancer risk perceptions across racial/ethnic groups although cancer screening is lower in nonwhite groups, and low PCR can be a barrier to screening and other cancer prevention behaviors. In the present study, we examined whether, in a nationally representative sample, nonwhites had lower PCR than whites and tested several possible explanations for these differences. Results indicated that racial/ethnic differences in awareness of family history, experience with risk behaviors (that is, smoking), and salience of cancer risk information could cause racial/ethnic differences in cancer risk perceptions; however, these results should be considered suggestive because we used correlational, cross-sectional data to test these pathways. Laboratory-based experimental and/or community-based quasi-experimental research is needed to confirm the causal pathways proposed on the basis of our findings. Fortunately, logical plausibility also lends support to the proposed pathways. It is reasonable that people who have never smoked might perceive their cancer risk to be lower; the reverse, that low PCR causes lower likelihood of being a smoker, is far less plausible. Similarly, although lower PCR could, to an extent, contribute to people being less attentive to information about, and less able to report their family history of cancer, this is probably less likely than knowing (or not knowing) that one has a family history of cancer affecting perceptions of risk. Finally, we proposed that racial/ethnic differences in the belief that everything causes cancer predict PCR. Although beliefs about the causes of cancer would seem to be antecedent to beliefs about risk perception, it is possible that the two constructs could be reciprocally related if global perceptions of risk shape how people process health information about cancer risk factors.
The study has several other limitations. Although the white, black, and Hispanic subsamples were relatively large, the Asian sample was relatively small (n = 181), resulting in relatively less stable estimates for comparisons between Asians and whites. In addition, ∼8% of the eligible cases were dropped from the analysis due to missing values on one or more of the key variables of interest. There were significant differences between participants who were dropped versus those retained with respect to demographic and other characteristics. We do not believe, however, that dropping these cases substantially affected the results of the study. If anything, they may have weakened the findings. Participants who were dropped were, on average, more likely to be nonwhite and perceive their risk to be lower than those who were retained.
Despite the constraints of using a data set not originally designed for the purpose of the present work, it was possible to identify several determinants of lower PCR among nonwhites compared with whites, perhaps most importantly, lower likelihood of reporting family history of cancer among nonwhites relative to whites. However, many additional hypotheses merit examination. Qualitative research has generated several possible explanations for why cancer risk perceptions are lower in black communities, including mistrust of a predominantly white medical establishment, lack of personally relevant cancer information, and lack of perceived personal control over health outcomes (31). This qualitative work ought to be extended to other nonwhite populations, and survey work is needed to test whether these explanations are born out in larger representative samples. Finally, we studied the perceptions of global cancer risk. Perhaps most of the pressing need is for research that examines racial/ethnic differences in PCR for specific cancers, including more fine-grained analyses of different aspects of risk (e.g., comparative versus absolute risk, risk of developing cancer versus risk of dying of cancer, or affective versus cognitive components of risk perception), and whether these risk perceptions are associated with cancer screening rates and prevention behavior.
Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest
No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.
Acknowledgments
We thank John Gaeddert for his assistance with the preparation of this manuscript.
Grant Support: During the preparation of this manuscript, supported was granted by NIH grant K07CA106225 (M.T. Kiviniemi) and by an Astella/AUA Foundation Rising Star in Urology Award (W. Underwood).
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked advertisement in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.